
Cognitive archaeology is a theoretical perspective in archaeology that focuses on the ancient mind. It is divided into two main groups: evolutionary cognitive archaeology (ECA), which seeks to understand human cognitive evolution from the material record, and ideational cognitive archaeology (ICA), which focuses on the symbolic structures discernable in or inferable from past material culture.
Evolutionary cognitive archaeology
ECA infers change in ancestral human cognition from the archaeological record, often drawing on the theories, methods, and data of other disciplines: cognitive science, comparative cognition, paleoneurology, experimental replication, and hands-on participation in the manufacture and use of traditional technologies. For example, the 3.3-million-year history of stone tool use is broadly informative of change in cognitive capacities like intelligence, spatial reasoning,working memory, and executive functioning, as defined by and understood through cognitive psychology and as operationalized to permit their detection in the archaeological record. Other ECA investigations have focused on the development of domain-specific abilities, including theory of mind,visual perception and visuospatial abilities, technological reasoning,language,numeracy, and literacy. ECA is broadly analogous to Steven Mithen's categories of cognitive-processual and evolutionary-cognitive archaeology.
Within ECA, there are two main schools of thought. The North American ECA school began in the mid-1970s with the pioneering work of archaeologist Thomas G. Wynn and biological anthropologist Sue Taylor Parker working with evolutionary neurobiologist Kathleen Gibson. It focuses on understanding human cognitive evolution, either from the artifactual record of forms like stone tools, comparisons of ancestral tool use with that of contemporary species (typically but not exclusively, non-human primates), or both. It often involves descriptive pattern analysis: analyzing change in a form like stone tools over millions of years and interpreting that change in terms of its cognitive significance using theories, constructs, and paradigms from cognitive psychology and neuroscience.
East of the Atlantic, the British ECA school also began in the mid-1970s with the work of archaeologists Colin Renfrew and John Gowlett and evolutionary primatologist William McGrew. Renfrew's work in particular, as well as that of his student, Lambros Malafouris, has taken a philosophical approach to the study of the ancient mind, drawing on concepts from the philosophy of mind and ecological psychology to examine the role of material structures in human cognition more fundamentally. Renfrew and Malafouris coined the term neuroarchaeology to describe their approach. ECA is concerned with how humans think through material structures, with the ability to leverage and exploit material structures for cognitive purposes perhaps being what truly sets human cognition apart from that of all other species. Pottery making is a typical example of this approach. Malafouris does not see the vase as a form created by the potter imposing an internal mental concept on external clay. Instead, the potter’s brain and body interact with his materials, the clay and the wheel; the form assumed by the clay is ultimately produced by the complex interaction between the potter’s perception of the feel of the clay, the pressure of his fingers on it, and its reactions of texture, moisture content, color, balance, and form.
Other early ECA pioneers include Glynn Isaac, archaeologist Iain Davidson, and psychologist William Noble. Today, ECA integrates interdisciplinary data from human psychology and neurophysiology, social anthropology, physical anthropology, comparative cognition, and artificial intelligence. As a vibrant and expanding field of inquiry,
"[ECA continues to] develop many of the same themes raised in the formative decade of cognitive archaeology: the validity and use of ethnoarchaeological and experimental methods; the question of continuities and discontinuities between humans and non-human species; the selection and application of theoretical frameworks, including the displacement of Piagetian theory by contemporary psychological and neuroscientific approaches to brain function and form; the incorporation of interdisciplinary data; the origin of language; the ability of construing intentionality from artifactual form; the philosophical turn in cognitive archaeology; and the riddle of intergenerational accumulation and transmission.": 6
Between 2018 and 2020, cognitive archaeologists Thomas Wynn and Lambros Malafouris headed a collaboration between the University of Colorado, Colorado Springs and the University of Oxford to examine the archaeology of the Lower Paleolithic through the lens of the extended mind; the results were published in the journal Adaptive Behavior in 2021.
Ideational cognitive archaeology
Archaeologist Thomas Huffman defined ideational cognitive archaeology as the study of prehistoric ideology: the ideals, values, and beliefs that constitute a society's worldview. It is analogous to the category Mithen called postprocessual cognitive archaeology.
"Archaeologists can tell from which mountain source a stone axe came, what minerals there are in a bronze bracelet, how old a dug-out canoe is. They can work out the probable cereal-yield from the fields of a Late Bronze Age farm. These are objective matters. But the language, laws, morals, religion of dead societies are different. They belong to the minds of man. Unless they were written down, and even then only if they were recorded accurately, we shall find it hard to recapture them."
ICA scholars often study the role that ideology and differing organizational approaches would have had on ancient peoples. The way that these abstract ideas are manifested through the remains these peoples have left can be investigated and debated often by drawing inferences and using approaches developed in fields such as semiotics, psychology and the wider sciences.
ICA uses the principles of sociocultural anthropology to investigate such diverse things as material symbols, the use of space, political power, and religion. For example, Huffman uses oral history sources from Zimbabwe and Portuguese documents to attempt to explain symbols discovered in the ruins of Great Zimbabwe, specifically connecting the Shona people's historical association of the right with men and the left with women to the placement of entrances to stone structures. Historian David Beach has pointed out that this ICA may be problematic in its logical leaps and incomplete use of archaeological sources, demonstrating the care that must be used when attempting to explain deep-time intentionality using archaeological evidence.
ICA also works with constructs such as the cognitive map. Humans do not behave under the influence of their senses alone but also through their past experiences such as their upbringing. These experiences contribute to each individual's unique view of the world, a kind of cognitive map that guides them. Groups of people living together tend to develop a shared view of the world and similar cognitive maps, which in turn influence their group material culture.
The multiple interpretations of an artifact, archaeological site or symbol are affected by the archaeologist's own experiences and ideas as well as those of the distant cultural tradition that created it. Cave art, for example, may not have been art in the modern sense at all, but was perhaps the product of ritual. Similarly, it would likely have described activities that were perfectly obvious to the people who created it, but the symbology employed will be different from that used today or at any other time.
Archaeologists have always tried to imagine what motivated people, but early efforts to understand how they thought were unstructured and speculative. Since the rise of processualism, these approaches have become more scientific, paying close attention to the archaeological context of finds and all possible interpretations. For example, a prehistoric bâton de commandement served an unknown purpose, but using ICA to interpret it would involve evaluating all its possible functions using clearly defined procedures and comparisons. By applying logic and experimental evidence, the most likely functions can be isolated.
It can also be argued that the material record shows behavioral traces that are the product of human thought, and thus would have been governed by a multitude of experiences and perspectives with the potential to influence behavior. The combination of material culture and actions can be further developed into a study of the ideas that drove action and used objects. This method attempts to avoid the pitfalls of post-processual archaeology by retaining the 'scientific' aspects of processual archaeology, while reaching for the higher social levels of ideas.
History of cognitive archaeology
Cognitive archaeology began in the 1970s as a reaction to the insistence of processual archaeology that the past be interpreted strictly according to the material evidence. This rigid materialism tended to limit archaeology to finding and describing artifacts, excluding broader interpretations of their possible cognitive and cultural significance as something beyond the reach of inferential reasoning. As social anthropologist Edmund Leach once put it, "all the ingenuity in the world will not replace the evidence that is lost and gone for ever," and "you should recognize your guesses for what they are.": 768
However, processual archaeology also opened up the possibility of investigating the lifestyle of those who made and used material culture. An initial approach was proposed by Lewis Binford, who suggested that ancient lifestyles could be understood by studying the traditional lifestyles of contemporary peoples. While this approach was subject to legitimate criticism, Binford's efforts nonetheless inspired further development of the idea that material forms could be informative about lifestyle, and as the product of intelligent behavior, might provide insight into how and perhaps even what their makers had thought. Archaeologists like Binford have also critiqued cognitive archaeology, stating it is only people's actions rather than their thoughts that are preserved in the archaeological record. ECA has responded to this criticism by stressing that it seeks to understand "how" ancient peoples thought using material structures, not "what" they thought.
Several early books helped popularize the idea that the ancient mind could be investigated and characterized, including Merlin Donald's Origins of the Modern Mind (1991), Steven Mithen's The Prehistory of Mind (1996), and David Lewis-Williams's The Mind in the Cave (2002).
See also
- Frederick L. Coolidge
- Merlin Donald
- David Lewis-Williams
- Lambros Malafouris
- Steven Mithen
- Karenleigh A. Overmann
- Colin Renfrew
- Thomas G. Wynn
- Neuroarchaeology
References
- Overmann, Karenleigh A; Coolidge, Frederick L (2019). "Cognitive Archaeology at the Crossroads". In Overmann, Karenleigh A; Coolidge, Frederick L (eds.). Squeezing Minds from Stones: Cognitive Archaeology and the Evolution of the Human Mind. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 1–12. ISBN 9780190854614.
- Harmand, Sonia; Lewis, Jason E; Feibel, Craig S; Lepre, Christopher J; Prat, Sandrine; Lenoble, Arnaud; Boës, Xavier; Quinn, Rhonda L; Brenet, Michel; Arroyo, Adrian; Taylor, Nicholas; Clément, Sophie; Guillaume, Daver; Brugal, Jean-Philippe; Leakey, Louise; Mortlock, Richard A; Wright, James D; Lokorodi, Sammy; Kirwa, Christopher; Kent, Dennis V; Roche, Hélène (2015). "3.3-Million-Year-Old Stone Tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya". Nature. 521 (7552): 310–315. Bibcode:2015Natur.521..310H. doi:10.1038/nature14464. PMID 25993961. S2CID 1207285.
- Wynn, Thomas (1979). "The Intelligence of Later Acheulean Hominids". Man. 14 (3): 371–391. doi:10.2307/2801865. JSTOR 2801865.
- Wynn, Thomas (1989). The Evolution of Spatial Competence. Chicago, IL: University of Illinois Press. ISBN 9780252060304.
- Coolidge, Frederick L; Wynn, Thomas (2001). "Executive Functions of the Frontal Lobes and the Evolutionary Ascendancy of Homo sapiens". Cambridge Archaeological Journal. 11 (3): 255–260. doi:10.1017/S0959774301000142.
- Coolidge, Frederick L; Wynn, Thomas (2005). "Working Memory, Its Executive Functions, and the Emergence of Modern Thinking". Cambridge Archaeological Journal. 15 (1): 5–26. doi:10.1017/S0959774305000016.
- Cole, James (2019). "Knapping in the Dark: Stone Tools and a Theory of Mind". In Overmann, Karenleigh A; Coolidge, Frederick L (eds.). Squeezing Minds from Stones: Cognitive Archaeology and the Evolution of the Human Mind. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 355–375. ISBN 9780190854614.
- Hodgson, Derek (2000). "Art, Perception and Information Processing: An Evolutionary Perspective". Rock Art Research. 17 (1): 3–34.
- Hodgson, Derek; Helvenston, Patricia A (2006). "The Emergence of the Representation of Animals in Palaeoart: Insights from Evolution and the Cognitive, Limbic and Visual Systems of the Human Brain". Rock Art Research. 23 (1): 3–40.
- Moore, Mark W (2011). "The Design Space of Stone Flaking: Implications for Cognitive Evolution". World Archaeology. 43 (4): 702–715. doi:10.1080/00438243.2011.624778. S2CID 161677366.
- Moore, Mark W; Perston, Yinika (2016). "Experimental Insights into the Cognitive Significance of Early Stone Tools". PLOS ONE. 11 (7): e0158803. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1158803M. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0158803. PMC 4938430. PMID 27392022.
- Putt, Shelby Stackhouse; Wijeakumar, Sobanawartiny; Franciscus, Robert G; Spencer, John P (2017). "The Functional Brain Networks That Underlie Early Stone Age Tool Manufacture". Nature Human Behaviour. 1 (6): 1–8. doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0102. S2CID 19830296.
- Overmann, Karenleigh A (2018). "Constructing a concept of number". Journal of Numerical Cognition. 4 (2): 464–493. doi:10.5964/jnc.v4i2.161. S2CID 52197209. Retrieved 10 July 2022.[permanent dead link ]
- Overmann, Karenleigh A. (2023). The Materiality of Numbers: Emergence and Elaboration from Prehistory to Present. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9781009361248.
- Overmann, Karenleigh A (2016). "Beyond writing: The development of literacy in the ancient Near East". Cambridge Archaeological Journal. 26 (2): 285–303. doi:10.1017/S0959774316000019. S2CID 163840618.
- Overmann, Karenleigh A (2022). "Early writing: A cognitive archaeological perspective on literacy and numeracy". Visible Language. 56 (1): 8–44. doi:10.34314/vl.v56i1.4934. Retrieved 10 July 2022.
- Overmann, Karenleigh A; Wynn, Thomas (2019). "Materiality and human cognition". Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory. 26 (2): 457–478. doi:10.1007/s10816-018-9378-y. S2CID 149605602.
- Mithen, Stephen J. (1999). "Cognitive archaeology". In Wilson, Robert A.; Keil, Frank C. (eds.). The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences. MIT Press. pp. 122–124. ISBN 9780262232005.
- Parker, Sue Taylor; Gibson, Kathleen R (1979). "A Developmental Model for the Evolution of Language and Intelligence in Early Hominids". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2 (3): 367–408. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0006307X.
- Renfrew, Colin (1982). Towards an Archaeology of Mind: An Inaugural Lecture Delivered before the University of Cambridge on 30th November 1982. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1080/00665983.1984.11077826.
- Renfrew, Colin (1994). "Towards a Cognitive Archaeology". In Renfrew, Colin; Zubrow, Ezra B W (eds.). In The Ancient Mind: Elements of Cognitive Archaeology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 3–12. ISBN 9780521456203.
- Gowlett, John A J (1979). "Complexities of Cultural Evidence in the Lower and Middle Pleistocene" (PDF). Nature. 278 (5699): 14–17. Bibcode:1979Natur.278...14G. doi:10.1038/278014b0. S2CID 4245666.
- Gowlett, John A J (1984). "Mental Abilities of Early Man: A Look at Some Hard Evidence". In Foley, Robert (ed.). Hominid Evolution and Community Ecology: Prehistoric Human Adaptation in Biological Perspective. London: Academic Press. pp. 167–192. OCLC 1014620102.
- McGrew, William Clement; Tutin, Caroline E G (1978). "Evidence for a Social Custom in Wild Chimpanzees?". Man. 13 (2): 234–251. doi:10.2307/2800247. JSTOR 2800247.
- McGrew, William Clement; Tutin, Caroline E G; Baldwin, Pamela J (1979). "Chimpanzees, Tools, and Termites: Cross-Cultural Comparisons of Senegal, Tanzania, and Rio Muni". Man. 14 (2): 185–215. doi:10.2307/2801563. JSTOR 2801563.
- Malafouris, Lambros; Renfrew, Colin, eds. (2010). The Cognitive Life of Things: Recasting the Boundaries of the Mind. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research. ISBN 9781902937519.
- Malafouris, Lambros (2013). How Things Shape the Mind: A Theory of Material Engagement. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 9780262528924.
- Renfrew, Colin; Malafouris, Lambros (2008). "Steps to a 'Neuroarchaeology' of Mind". Cambridge Archaeological Journal. 18 (3): 381–385. doi:10.1017/S0959774308000425. S2CID 231810895.
- Malafouris, Lambros (2010). "Metaplasticity and the Human Becoming: Principles of Neuroarchaeology" (PDF). Journal of Anthropological Sciences. 88: 49–72. PMID 20834050.
- Overmann, Karenleigh A (2021). "The Material Difference in Human Cognition". Adaptive Behavior. 29 (2): 123–136. doi:10.1177/1059712320930738. hdl:11250/2766177.
- Malafouris, Lambros (2008). "At the potter's wheel: An argument for material agency". In Malafouris, Lambros; Knappett, Carl (eds.). Material Agency: Towards a Non-Anthropocentric Approach. New York: Springer Science+Business Media. pp. 19–36. OCLC 859423170.
- Isaac, Glynn Llywelyn (1976). "Stages of Cultural Elaboration in the Pleistocene: Possible Archaeological Indicators of the Development of Language Capabilities". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 280 (1): 275–288. Bibcode:1976NYASA.280..275I. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1976.tb25494.x. S2CID 84642536.
- Isaac, Glynn Llywelyn (1984). "The Archaeology of Human Origins: Studies of the Lower Pleistocene in East Africa 1971–1981". In Wendorf, Fred; Close, Angela E (eds.). Advances in World Archaeology. New York: Academic Press. pp. 1–87. ISBN 9780120399017.
- Davidson, Iain; Noble, William (1989). "The Archaeology of Perception: Traces of Depiction and Language". Current Anthropology. 30 (2): 125–155. doi:10.1086/203723. JSTOR 2743542. S2CID 144094302.
- Noble, William; Davidson, Iain (1996). Human Evolution, Language, and Mind: A Psychological and Archaeological Inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521445023.
- Wynn, Thomas; Overmann, Karenleigh A; Malafouris, Lambros (2021). "4E cognition in the Lower Paleolithic: An introduction". Adaptive Behavior. 29 (2): 99–106. doi:10.1177/1059712320967184.
- Huffman, Thomas (1986). "Cognitive studies of the iron age in Southern Africa". World Archaeology. 18: 84–95. doi:10.1080/00438243.1986.9979990.
- Burl, Aubrey (1981). Rites of the Gods. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson. ISBN 978-0460043137.
- Beach, David (1998). "Cognitive Archaeology and Imaginary History at Great Zimbabwe". Current Anthropology. 39: 47–72. doi:10.1086/204698. S2CID 143970768.
- Hawkes, Christopher (1954). "Archeological Theory and Method: Some Suggestions from the Old World". American Anthropologist. 56 (2): 155–168. doi:10.1525/aa.1954.56.2.02a00020. JSTOR 664357.
- Leach, Edmund R (1973). "Concluding Address". In Renfrew, Colin (ed.). The Explanation of Culture Change: Models in Prehistory. Proceedings of a Meeting of the Research Seminar in Archaeology and Related Subjects Held at the University of Sheffield, December 14–16, 1971. London: Gerald Duckworth. pp. 761–771. OCLC 476129218.
- Binford, Lewis R (1962). "Archaeology as Anthropology". American Antiquity. 28 (2): 217–225. doi:10.2307/278380. JSTOR 278380. S2CID 162242910.
- Binford, Lewis R (1972). An Archaeological Perspective. New York: Seminar Press. ISBN 9780127850535.
- Donald, Merlin (1991). Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 9780674644847.
- Mithen, Steven J (1996). The Prehistory of Mind: The Cognitive Origins of Art, Religion and Science. London: Thames & Hudson. ISBN 9780500281000.
- Lewis-Williams, David (2002). The Mind in the Cave: Consciousness and the Origins of Art. London: Thames & Hudson. ISBN 0-500-05117-8.
Further reading
- Cognitive Archaeology
- AURA Homepage of Cognitive Archaeology
- 4E Cognition in the Lower Palaeolithic
External links
- University of Colorado, Colorado Springs Center for Cognitive Archaeology
- Rock Art Research Institute, University of Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa
Cognitive archaeology is a theoretical perspective in archaeology that focuses on the ancient mind It is divided into two main groups evolutionary cognitive archaeology ECA which seeks to understand human cognitive evolution from the material record and ideational cognitive archaeology ICA which focuses on the symbolic structures discernable in or inferable from past material culture Evolutionary cognitive archaeologyECA infers change in ancestral human cognition from the archaeological record often drawing on the theories methods and data of other disciplines cognitive science comparative cognition paleoneurology experimental replication and hands on participation in the manufacture and use of traditional technologies For example the 3 3 million year history of stone tool use is broadly informative of change in cognitive capacities like intelligence spatial reasoning working memory and executive functioning as defined by and understood through cognitive psychology and as operationalized to permit their detection in the archaeological record Other ECA investigations have focused on the development of domain specific abilities including theory of mind visual perception and visuospatial abilities technological reasoning language numeracy and literacy ECA is broadly analogous to Steven Mithen s categories of cognitive processual and evolutionary cognitive archaeology Within ECA there are two main schools of thought The North American ECA school began in the mid 1970s with the pioneering work of archaeologist Thomas G Wynn and biological anthropologist Sue Taylor Parker working with evolutionary neurobiologist Kathleen Gibson It focuses on understanding human cognitive evolution either from the artifactual record of forms like stone tools comparisons of ancestral tool use with that of contemporary species typically but not exclusively non human primates or both It often involves descriptive pattern analysis analyzing change in a form like stone tools over millions of years and interpreting that change in terms of its cognitive significance using theories constructs and paradigms from cognitive psychology and neuroscience East of the Atlantic the British ECA school also began in the mid 1970s with the work of archaeologists Colin Renfrew and John Gowlett and evolutionary primatologist William McGrew Renfrew s work in particular as well as that of his student Lambros Malafouris has taken a philosophical approach to the study of the ancient mind drawing on concepts from the philosophy of mind and ecological psychology to examine the role of material structures in human cognition more fundamentally Renfrew and Malafouris coined the term neuroarchaeology to describe their approach ECA is concerned with how humans think through material structures with the ability to leverage and exploit material structures for cognitive purposes perhaps being what truly sets human cognition apart from that of all other species Pottery making is a typical example of this approach Malafouris does not see the vase as a form created by the potter imposing an internal mental concept on external clay Instead the potter s brain and body interact with his materials the clay and the wheel the form assumed by the clay is ultimately produced by the complex interaction between the potter s perception of the feel of the clay the pressure of his fingers on it and its reactions of texture moisture content color balance and form Other early ECA pioneers include Glynn Isaac archaeologist Iain Davidson and psychologist William Noble Today ECA integrates interdisciplinary data from human psychology and neurophysiology social anthropology physical anthropology comparative cognition and artificial intelligence As a vibrant and expanding field of inquiry ECA continues to develop many of the same themes raised in the formative decade of cognitive archaeology the validity and use of ethnoarchaeological and experimental methods the question of continuities and discontinuities between humans and non human species the selection and application of theoretical frameworks including the displacement of Piagetian theory by contemporary psychological and neuroscientific approaches to brain function and form the incorporation of interdisciplinary data the origin of language the ability of construing intentionality from artifactual form the philosophical turn in cognitive archaeology and the riddle of intergenerational accumulation and transmission 6 Between 2018 and 2020 cognitive archaeologists Thomas Wynn and Lambros Malafouris headed a collaboration between the University of Colorado Colorado Springs and the University of Oxford to examine the archaeology of the Lower Paleolithic through the lens of the extended mind the results were published in the journal Adaptive Behavior in 2021 Ideational cognitive archaeologyArchaeologist Thomas Huffman defined ideational cognitive archaeology as the study of prehistoric ideology the ideals values and beliefs that constitute a society s worldview It is analogous to the category Mithen called postprocessual cognitive archaeology Archaeologists can tell from which mountain source a stone axe came what minerals there are in a bronze bracelet how old a dug out canoe is They can work out the probable cereal yield from the fields of a Late Bronze Age farm These are objective matters But the language laws morals religion of dead societies are different They belong to the minds of man Unless they were written down and even then only if they were recorded accurately we shall find it hard to recapture them Aubrey Burl Rites of the Gods 1981 p 15 ICA scholars often study the role that ideology and differing organizational approaches would have had on ancient peoples The way that these abstract ideas are manifested through the remains these peoples have left can be investigated and debated often by drawing inferences and using approaches developed in fields such as semiotics psychology and the wider sciences ICA uses the principles of sociocultural anthropology to investigate such diverse things as material symbols the use of space political power and religion For example Huffman uses oral history sources from Zimbabwe and Portuguese documents to attempt to explain symbols discovered in the ruins of Great Zimbabwe specifically connecting the Shona people s historical association of the right with men and the left with women to the placement of entrances to stone structures Historian David Beach has pointed out that this ICA may be problematic in its logical leaps and incomplete use of archaeological sources demonstrating the care that must be used when attempting to explain deep time intentionality using archaeological evidence ICA also works with constructs such as the cognitive map Humans do not behave under the influence of their senses alone but also through their past experiences such as their upbringing These experiences contribute to each individual s unique view of the world a kind of cognitive map that guides them Groups of people living together tend to develop a shared view of the world and similar cognitive maps which in turn influence their group material culture The multiple interpretations of an artifact archaeological site or symbol are affected by the archaeologist s own experiences and ideas as well as those of the distant cultural tradition that created it Cave art for example may not have been art in the modern sense at all but was perhaps the product of ritual Similarly it would likely have described activities that were perfectly obvious to the people who created it but the symbology employed will be different from that used today or at any other time Archaeologists have always tried to imagine what motivated people but early efforts to understand how they thought were unstructured and speculative Since the rise of processualism these approaches have become more scientific paying close attention to the archaeological context of finds and all possible interpretations For example a prehistoric baton de commandement served an unknown purpose but using ICA to interpret it would involve evaluating all its possible functions using clearly defined procedures and comparisons By applying logic and experimental evidence the most likely functions can be isolated It can also be argued that the material record shows behavioral traces that are the product of human thought and thus would have been governed by a multitude of experiences and perspectives with the potential to influence behavior The combination of material culture and actions can be further developed into a study of the ideas that drove action and used objects This method attempts to avoid the pitfalls of post processual archaeology by retaining the scientific aspects of processual archaeology while reaching for the higher social levels of ideas History of cognitive archaeologyCognitive archaeology began in the 1970s as a reaction to the insistence of processual archaeology that the past be interpreted strictly according to the material evidence This rigid materialism tended to limit archaeology to finding and describing artifacts excluding broader interpretations of their possible cognitive and cultural significance as something beyond the reach of inferential reasoning As social anthropologist Edmund Leach once put it all the ingenuity in the world will not replace the evidence that is lost and gone for ever and you should recognize your guesses for what they are 768 However processual archaeology also opened up the possibility of investigating the lifestyle of those who made and used material culture An initial approach was proposed by Lewis Binford who suggested that ancient lifestyles could be understood by studying the traditional lifestyles of contemporary peoples While this approach was subject to legitimate criticism Binford s efforts nonetheless inspired further development of the idea that material forms could be informative about lifestyle and as the product of intelligent behavior might provide insight into how and perhaps even what their makers had thought Archaeologists like Binford have also critiqued cognitive archaeology stating it is only people s actions rather than their thoughts that are preserved in the archaeological record ECA has responded to this criticism by stressing that it seeks to understand how ancient peoples thought using material structures not what they thought Several early books helped popularize the idea that the ancient mind could be investigated and characterized including Merlin Donald s Origins of the Modern Mind 1991 Steven Mithen s The Prehistory of Mind 1996 and David Lewis Williams s The Mind in the Cave 2002 See alsoFrederick L Coolidge Merlin Donald David Lewis Williams Lambros Malafouris Steven Mithen Karenleigh A Overmann Colin Renfrew Thomas G Wynn NeuroarchaeologyReferencesOvermann Karenleigh A Coolidge Frederick L 2019 Cognitive Archaeology at the Crossroads In Overmann Karenleigh A Coolidge Frederick L eds Squeezing Minds from Stones Cognitive Archaeology and the Evolution of the Human Mind New York Oxford University Press pp 1 12 ISBN 9780190854614 Harmand Sonia Lewis Jason E Feibel Craig S Lepre Christopher J Prat Sandrine Lenoble Arnaud Boes Xavier Quinn Rhonda L Brenet Michel Arroyo Adrian Taylor Nicholas Clement Sophie Guillaume Daver Brugal Jean Philippe Leakey Louise Mortlock Richard A Wright James D Lokorodi Sammy Kirwa Christopher Kent Dennis V Roche Helene 2015 3 3 Million Year Old Stone Tools from Lomekwi 3 West Turkana Kenya Nature 521 7552 310 315 Bibcode 2015Natur 521 310H doi 10 1038 nature14464 PMID 25993961 S2CID 1207285 Wynn Thomas 1979 The Intelligence of Later Acheulean Hominids Man 14 3 371 391 doi 10 2307 2801865 JSTOR 2801865 Wynn Thomas 1989 The Evolution of Spatial Competence Chicago IL University of Illinois Press ISBN 9780252060304 Coolidge Frederick L Wynn Thomas 2001 Executive Functions of the Frontal Lobes and the Evolutionary Ascendancy of Homo sapiens Cambridge Archaeological Journal 11 3 255 260 doi 10 1017 S0959774301000142 Coolidge Frederick L Wynn Thomas 2005 Working Memory Its Executive Functions and the Emergence of Modern Thinking Cambridge Archaeological Journal 15 1 5 26 doi 10 1017 S0959774305000016 Cole James 2019 Knapping in the Dark Stone Tools and a Theory of Mind In Overmann Karenleigh A Coolidge Frederick L eds Squeezing Minds from Stones Cognitive Archaeology and the Evolution of the Human Mind New York Oxford University Press pp 355 375 ISBN 9780190854614 Hodgson Derek 2000 Art Perception and Information Processing An Evolutionary Perspective Rock Art Research 17 1 3 34 Hodgson Derek Helvenston Patricia A 2006 The Emergence of the Representation of Animals in Palaeoart Insights from Evolution and the Cognitive Limbic and Visual Systems of the Human Brain Rock Art Research 23 1 3 40 Moore Mark W 2011 The Design Space of Stone Flaking Implications for Cognitive Evolution World Archaeology 43 4 702 715 doi 10 1080 00438243 2011 624778 S2CID 161677366 Moore Mark W Perston Yinika 2016 Experimental Insights into the Cognitive Significance of Early Stone Tools PLOS ONE 11 7 e0158803 Bibcode 2016PLoSO 1158803M doi 10 1371 journal pone 0158803 PMC 4938430 PMID 27392022 Putt Shelby Stackhouse Wijeakumar Sobanawartiny Franciscus Robert G Spencer John P 2017 The Functional Brain Networks That Underlie Early Stone Age Tool Manufacture Nature Human Behaviour 1 6 1 8 doi 10 1038 s41562 017 0102 S2CID 19830296 Overmann Karenleigh A 2018 Constructing a concept of number Journal of Numerical Cognition 4 2 464 493 doi 10 5964 jnc v4i2 161 S2CID 52197209 Retrieved 10 July 2022 permanent dead link Overmann Karenleigh A 2023 The Materiality of Numbers Emergence and Elaboration from Prehistory to Present Cambridge University Press ISBN 9781009361248 Overmann Karenleigh A 2016 Beyond writing The development of literacy in the ancient Near East Cambridge Archaeological Journal 26 2 285 303 doi 10 1017 S0959774316000019 S2CID 163840618 Overmann Karenleigh A 2022 Early writing A cognitive archaeological perspective on literacy and numeracy Visible Language 56 1 8 44 doi 10 34314 vl v56i1 4934 Retrieved 10 July 2022 Overmann Karenleigh A Wynn Thomas 2019 Materiality and human cognition Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 26 2 457 478 doi 10 1007 s10816 018 9378 y S2CID 149605602 Mithen Stephen J 1999 Cognitive archaeology In Wilson Robert A Keil Frank C eds The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences MIT Press pp 122 124 ISBN 9780262232005 Parker Sue Taylor Gibson Kathleen R 1979 A Developmental Model for the Evolution of Language and Intelligence in Early Hominids Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 3 367 408 doi 10 1017 S0140525X0006307X Renfrew Colin 1982 Towards an Archaeology of Mind An Inaugural Lecture Delivered before the University of Cambridge on 30th November 1982 Cambridge Cambridge University Press doi 10 1080 00665983 1984 11077826 Renfrew Colin 1994 Towards a Cognitive Archaeology In Renfrew Colin Zubrow Ezra B W eds In The Ancient Mind Elements of Cognitive Archaeology Cambridge Cambridge University Press pp 3 12 ISBN 9780521456203 Gowlett John A J 1979 Complexities of Cultural Evidence in the Lower and Middle Pleistocene PDF Nature 278 5699 14 17 Bibcode 1979Natur 278 14G doi 10 1038 278014b0 S2CID 4245666 Gowlett John A J 1984 Mental Abilities of Early Man A Look at Some Hard Evidence In Foley Robert ed Hominid Evolution and Community Ecology Prehistoric Human Adaptation in Biological Perspective London Academic Press pp 167 192 OCLC 1014620102 McGrew William Clement Tutin Caroline E G 1978 Evidence for a Social Custom in Wild Chimpanzees Man 13 2 234 251 doi 10 2307 2800247 JSTOR 2800247 McGrew William Clement Tutin Caroline E G Baldwin Pamela J 1979 Chimpanzees Tools and Termites Cross Cultural Comparisons of Senegal Tanzania and Rio Muni Man 14 2 185 215 doi 10 2307 2801563 JSTOR 2801563 Malafouris Lambros Renfrew Colin eds 2010 The Cognitive Life of Things Recasting the Boundaries of the Mind Cambridge McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research ISBN 9781902937519 Malafouris Lambros 2013 How Things Shape the Mind A Theory of Material Engagement Cambridge MA MIT Press ISBN 9780262528924 Renfrew Colin Malafouris Lambros 2008 Steps to a Neuroarchaeology of Mind Cambridge Archaeological Journal 18 3 381 385 doi 10 1017 S0959774308000425 S2CID 231810895 Malafouris Lambros 2010 Metaplasticity and the Human Becoming Principles of Neuroarchaeology PDF Journal of Anthropological Sciences 88 49 72 PMID 20834050 Overmann Karenleigh A 2021 The Material Difference in Human Cognition Adaptive Behavior 29 2 123 136 doi 10 1177 1059712320930738 hdl 11250 2766177 Malafouris Lambros 2008 At the potter s wheel An argument for material agency In Malafouris Lambros Knappett Carl eds Material Agency Towards a Non Anthropocentric Approach New York Springer Science Business Media pp 19 36 OCLC 859423170 Isaac Glynn Llywelyn 1976 Stages of Cultural Elaboration in the Pleistocene Possible Archaeological Indicators of the Development of Language Capabilities Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 280 1 275 288 Bibcode 1976NYASA 280 275I doi 10 1111 j 1749 6632 1976 tb25494 x S2CID 84642536 Isaac Glynn Llywelyn 1984 The Archaeology of Human Origins Studies of the Lower Pleistocene in East Africa 1971 1981 In Wendorf Fred Close Angela E eds Advances in World Archaeology New York Academic Press pp 1 87 ISBN 9780120399017 Davidson Iain Noble William 1989 The Archaeology of Perception Traces of Depiction and Language Current Anthropology 30 2 125 155 doi 10 1086 203723 JSTOR 2743542 S2CID 144094302 Noble William Davidson Iain 1996 Human Evolution Language and Mind A Psychological and Archaeological Inquiry Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 9780521445023 Wynn Thomas Overmann Karenleigh A Malafouris Lambros 2021 4E cognition in the Lower Paleolithic An introduction Adaptive Behavior 29 2 99 106 doi 10 1177 1059712320967184 Huffman Thomas 1986 Cognitive studies of the iron age in Southern Africa World Archaeology 18 84 95 doi 10 1080 00438243 1986 9979990 Burl Aubrey 1981 Rites of the Gods London Weidenfeld amp Nicolson ISBN 978 0460043137 Beach David 1998 Cognitive Archaeology and Imaginary History at Great Zimbabwe Current Anthropology 39 47 72 doi 10 1086 204698 S2CID 143970768 Hawkes Christopher 1954 Archeological Theory and Method Some Suggestions from the Old World American Anthropologist 56 2 155 168 doi 10 1525 aa 1954 56 2 02a00020 JSTOR 664357 Leach Edmund R 1973 Concluding Address In Renfrew Colin ed The Explanation of Culture Change Models in Prehistory Proceedings of a Meeting of the Research Seminar in Archaeology and Related Subjects Held at the University of Sheffield December 14 16 1971 London Gerald Duckworth pp 761 771 OCLC 476129218 Binford Lewis R 1962 Archaeology as Anthropology American Antiquity 28 2 217 225 doi 10 2307 278380 JSTOR 278380 S2CID 162242910 Binford Lewis R 1972 An Archaeological Perspective New York Seminar Press ISBN 9780127850535 Donald Merlin 1991 Origins of the Modern Mind Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition Cambridge MA Harvard University Press ISBN 9780674644847 Mithen Steven J 1996 The Prehistory of Mind The Cognitive Origins of Art Religion and Science London Thames amp Hudson ISBN 9780500281000 Lewis Williams David 2002 The Mind in the Cave Consciousness and the Origins of Art London Thames amp Hudson ISBN 0 500 05117 8 Further readingCognitive Archaeology AURA Homepage of Cognitive Archaeology 4E Cognition in the Lower PalaeolithicExternal linksUniversity of Colorado Colorado Springs Center for Cognitive Archaeology Rock Art Research Institute University of Witwatersrand Johannesburg South Africa